follow CCP

Recent blog entries
popular papers

Science Curiosity and Political Information Processing

What Is the "Science of Science Communication"?

Climate-Science Communication and the Measurement Problem

Ideology, Motivated Cognition, and Cognitive Reflection: An Experimental Study

'Ideology' or 'Situation Sense'? An Experimental Investigation of Motivated Reasoning and Professional Judgment

A Risky Science Communication Environment for Vaccines

Motivated Numeracy and Enlightened Self-Government

Ideology, Motivated Cognition, and Cognitive Reflection: An Experimental Study

Making Climate Science Communication Evidence-based—All the Way Down 

Neutral Principles, Motivated Cognition, and Some Problems for Constitutional Law 

Cultural Cognition of Scientific Consensus
 

The Tragedy of the Risk-Perception Commons: Science Literacy and Climate Change

"They Saw a Protest": Cognitive Illiberalism and the Speech-Conduct Distinction 

Geoengineering and the Science Communication Environment: a Cross-Cultural Experiment

Fixing the Communications Failure

Why We Are Poles Apart on Climate Change

The Cognitively Illiberal State 

Who Fears the HPV Vaccine, Who Doesn't, and Why? An Experimental Study

Cultural Cognition of the Risks and Benefits of Nanotechnology

Whose Eyes Are You Going to Believe? An Empirical Examination of Scott v. Harris

Cultural Cognition and Public Policy

Culture, Cognition, and Consent: Who Perceives What, and Why, in "Acquaintance Rape" Cases

Culture and Identity-Protective Cognition: Explaining the White Male Effect

Fear of Democracy: A Cultural Evaluation of Sunstein on Risk

Cultural Cognition as a Conception of the Cultural Theory of Risk

« New "strongest evidence yet" on consensus messaging! | Main | "Now I'm here ... now I'm there ...": If you look, our dualistic identity-expressive/science-knowledge-acquiring selves go through only one slit »
Monday
Apr182016

First things first: the science of normal (nonpathological) science communication

From something I'm working on...

The priority of the science of normal science science communication 

The source of nearly every science-communication misadventure can be traced to a single mistake: the confusion of the processes that make science valid  for the ones that vouch for the validity of it.  As Popper (1960) noted, it is naïve, to view the “truth as manifest” even after it has been ascertained by science. The scientific knowledge that individuals rely on in the course of their everyday lives is far too voluminous, far too specialized for any—including a scientist—to comprehend or verify for herself.  So how do people manage to pull it off?  What are social cues they rely on to distinguish the currency of scientific knowledge from the myriad counterfeit alternatives to it? What processes generate those cues? What are the cognitive faculties that determine how proficiently individuals are able to recognize and interpret them? Most importantly of all, how do the answers to these questions vary--as they must in modern democratic societies--across communities of culturally diverse citizens, whose members are immersed in a plurality of parallel systems suited for enabling them to identify who knows what about what? These questions not only admit of scientific inquiry; they demand it.  Unless we understand how ordinary members of the public ordinarily do manage to converge on the best available evidence, we will never fully understand why they occasionally do not, and what can be done to combat these noxious sources of ignorance.

 

Reference

Popper, K.R. On the Sources of Knowledge and of Ignorance. in Conjectures and Refutations 3-40 (Oxford University Press London, 1960).

 


PrintView Printer Friendly Version

EmailEmail Article to Friend

References (1)

References allow you to track sources for this article, as well as articles that were written in response to this article.

Reader Comments (2)

I propose that the story will be more palatable and memorable to the public if we also ask how scientists and specialists ordinarily do manage to converge on the best available evidence.
Which is to ask "what do scientists do when they find themselves in our shoes?".

My theory is that there are a number of parallels.
-------------

I recall reading a comment from a sociology of science study which indicated that practicing scientists tend to be most skeptical of "findings", data, and theories from their own field of specialty. One could draw several implications from that --- one being that scientists tend to lose their sense of "health skepticism" the further away they get from their own fields.

April 24, 2016 | Unregistered CommenterCortlandt

I believe looking at scientists, and intra-scientific appraisal, offers a particularly appealing way to start into the general problem of public evidence convergence- in large part because folks like Popper have prescribed criteria for the evaluation of evidence, and because others like Lakatos have articulated how that convergence is affected by social factors.

That said, I think I disagree with you about the specific case. My inclination is to be much more skeptical about the belief formation habits of scientists than you appear to be in this post. My suspicion is that in response to the specialization and fragmentation of scientific knowledge (Popper's Babel of Tongues problem), and other pressures, scientists are reliant on heuristic cues to evidentiary quality that are not/i> valid. This is roughly why we wind up having replication crises in psychology and John Ionnaidis standing on the quad wearing a sandwich board saying "The end is nigh".

Although these sources of appraisal error may not be particularly large, such errors may also not be randomly distributed- and not easily remedied by the procedures of critical self-correction which scientific discourse is meant to foster. Obvious examples would include many of the typical features of the heuristics literature, such as source errors, but also more sophisticated problems of the sort that you've already written about here- widespread misunderstandings of basic statistics and reporting, or field-specific culturally motivated processing of disputes (The behaviorism debate is a fun example in psych for that one, or if you'd prefer a physical cultural locus, debates in economics between different salinities).

April 30, 2016 | Unregistered CommenterRobert Marriott

PostPost a New Comment

Enter your information below to add a new comment.

My response is on my own website »
Author Email (optional):
Author URL (optional):
Post:
 
Some HTML allowed: <a href="" title=""> <abbr title=""> <acronym title=""> <b> <blockquote cite=""> <code> <em> <i> <strike> <strong>